Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Acquisition of chemical recognition cues facilitates integration into ant societies

Abstract

Background

Social insects maintain the integrity of their societies by discriminating between colony members and foreigners through cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) signatures. Nevertheless, parasites frequently get access to social resources, for example through mimicry of host CHCs among other mechanisms. The origin of mimetic compounds, however, remains unknown in the majority of studies (biosynthesis vs. acquisition). Additionally, direct evidence is scarce that chemical mimicry is indeed beneficial to the parasites (e.g., by improving social acceptance).

Results

In the present study we demonstrated that the kleptoparasitic silverfish Malayatelura ponerophila most likely acquires CHCs directly from its host ant Leptogenys distinguenda by evaluating the transfer of a stable-isotope label from the cuticle of workers to the silverfish. In a second experiment, we prevented CHC pilfering by separating silverfish from their host for six or nine days. Chemical host resemblance as well as aggressive rejection behaviour by host ants was then quantified for unmanipulated and previously separated individuals. Separated individuals showed reduced chemical host resemblance and they received significantly more aggressive rejection behaviour than unmanipulated individuals.

Conclusion

Our study clarifies the mechanism of chemical mimicry in a social insect parasite in great detail. It shows empirically for the first time that social insect parasites are able to acquire CHCs from their host. Furthermore, it demonstrates that the accuracy of chemical mimicry can be crucial for social insect parasites by enhancing social acceptance and, thus, allowing successful exploitation. We discuss the results in the light of coevolutionary arms races between parasites and hosts.

Background

Host-parasite interactions are often regarded as coevolutionary "arms races" in which a host and a parasite species exert reciprocal selection pressures on one another over long periods of time [1]. Under coevolution, parasite species adapt towards encountering a host and exploiting it successfully, whereas host species in turn adapt towards an avoidance of parasite encounters or a successful defence against them [2]. Accordingly, hosts have evolved a great variety of defence mechanisms to prevent all sorts of exploitative attacks [3, 4].

Since social insects are widespread and extraordinarily abundant [5] they are subject to exploitation. As a consequence, they have evolved sophisticated recognition systems to protect their colonies, their brood, and their resources from competitors, predators and parasites, thereby maintaining the integrity of their societies [6]. The recognition of group members in social insects is mainly based on chemical cues [7–9]. Individuals compare the chemical cues expressed by a counterpart with an internal template, which is the chemical signature expected in all members of the society [10]. Complex blends of cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) seem to comprise all essential information necessary for nestmate recognition in ants, wasps and termites [11]. Due to effective recognition systems, invaders are frequently recognized, attacked, expelled or even killed by social insect workers.

Nevertheless, a multitude of organisms, particularly invertebrates, are known to exploit social insect societies [12–14], for example, by preying directly on the host, by stealing their food, or merely by inhabiting a well-protected habitat with a stable microclimate [6]. Many of these organisms, commonly known as myrmecophiles, are more or less permanently associated with ant colonies [15]. However, tight associations with ants require specific adaptations, that is, intruders must be able to invade host colonies and maintain contact without being expelled or killed. Some species not only manage to invade ant societies successfully, they also remain permanently integrated [6, 16, 17] in a way that the hosts behave amicably to the intruders as if they were part of their society [18].

A range of specific strategies exist to penetrate ant societies, and eventually to remain permanently integrated, including chemical, acoustic, morphological and behavioural adaptations [6, 10, 19–22]. Chemical strategies are particularly widespread among myrmecophiles, most likely because ants rely strongly on chemical communication [5, 23]. Several chemical strategies have been described, such as chemical mimicry (chemical resemblance of another species), chemical camouflage/crypsis (avoiding detection through expression of uninteresting or background cues), chemical insignificance (suppression of chemical recognition cues) or the use of ant deterrent/attractant chemicals [10, 19]. Pretending to be a member of the colony by mimicking the ants' CHCs (chemical mimicry) is among the most frequent chemical strategies among myrmecophiles [10, 23]. Although another definition of chemical mimicry exists [24], we use this term consistently with its original biological definition according to Dettner & Liepert [19], irrespective of the mechanism through which these mimetic compounds are acquired. Nevertheless, to include this information, we consider chemical mimicry to be either innate (biosynthesis of compounds), acquired (adoption of compounds), or mixed.

Regarding the origins of mimetic compounds, we assume that the acquisition as well as the innate production of ant CHCs may be associated with costs for the myrmecophiles. As expected from a trade-off model, such costs must be balanced by a benefit of performing chemical mimicry [25], and this has rarely been tested empirically. In numerous cases, chemical mimicry presumably works through acquisition of host odours through physical contact rather than through biosynthesis [19, 23]. However, the origin of mimetic compounds remains unclear in the majority of cases. As in many other examples, the kleptoparasitic silverfish Malayatelura ponerophila (Zygentoma, Atelurinae; Figure 1) was found to resemble the CHC profiles of its Southeast Asian army ant host, Leptogenys distinguenda. A closer analysis suggested the acquisition of host cues through physical contact, as the silverfish was observed to interact frequently with its host through rubbing its surface on that of host ant workers [26]. Nevertheless, final proof was lacking so that the mechanism remained speculative and a biosynthesis of mimetic cues could not be completely ruled out, as is the case in most other examples.

Figure 1
figure 1

Interactions of M. ponerophila with host workers. (A) The silverfish is frequently found beneath host ant workers. Physical contact with the host ants allows the silverfish to acquire cuticular hydrocarbons, which are used by ants as recognition cues. (B) Life in ant colonies also entails a high risk for silverfish, as they are sometimes recognised, attacked, and killed by the host ants. © C. von Beeren.

The aims of the present study were twofold; 1) to clarify the underlying mechanism of chemical mimicry (innate vs. acquired), and 2) to test whether a good match of chemical host recognition cues is in fact beneficial to the mimic (e.g., by facilitating social integration). Two experimental approaches were used to address these questions. First, we marked host ant workers with a stable isotope-labelled hydrocarbon and monitored the transfer of this artificial label to the myrmecophilous silverfish. If M. ponerophila acquires CHCs from its host, we expected that the label would also accumulate on their cuticles. Second, we aimed to experimentally reduce the chemical host resemblance of silverfish individuals in order to study the effects on social integration. Therefore, silverfish were isolated from their host ants for six and nine days, respectively. Under the assumption of a behavioural acquisition of host recognition cues by the parasite, the silverfish were expected to lose host CHCs over time when isolated, resulting in reduced chemical similarity to the host. This assumption was checked by analysing cuticular chemical profiles of isolated and non-isolated silverfish and comparing them to their host. We expected that silverfish exhibiting reduced chemical host resemblance would be less socially integrated as a consequence of being increasingly recognized as alien. Hence, we tested through behavioural studies whether isolated silverfish (with reduced host similarity) were attacked more frequently than unmanipulated individuals.

Methods

(a) Field collection and animal maintenance

Animals were collected and observed at the Field Studies Centre of the University Malaya in Ulu Gombak, Malaysia (03°19.479' N, 101°45.163' E, altitude 230 m) and at the Institute of Biodiversity in Bukit Rengit, Malaysia (03°35.779' N, 102°10.814' E, altitude 72 m). Ten months of field work were carried out in total between August 2008 and April 2011. We searched for L. distinguenda raiding trails during the night, tracked them back to their bivouac-like nests and subsequently checked every 30 min between 8 p.m. and 3 a.m. for the onset of a migration. The silverfish Malayatelura ponerophila [27] participate in migrations either by phoretic transport on host pupae or by following the ants' pheromone trail on their own [28]. The collected animals were either extracted directly for subsequent chemical analysis of CHCs, or they were maintained in artificial laboratory nests for various experiments (see below). Experimental colonies were assembled differently depending on the numbers of collected individuals and on the experimental protocol. Laboratory nests contained only members from one particular host colony (i.e., colonies were never mixed). If not described differently, nests consisted of a clear plastic container (20 × 14 × 1 cm), shaded with a plastic cover, and with a 1.5 cm wide entrance. The nest was placed in a larger foraging arena (32 × 25 × 9 cm) with a moistened plaster floor to maintain constant humidity. For isolation experiments animals were also kept separated from their home colonies in plastic containers (20 × 14 × 5 cm) equipped with a moist plaster floor. Animals in laboratory nests and those in isolation were fed every day with freshly killed crickets. Crickets are among the natural diet of the host ants [29] and the silverfish (personal observation). All behavioural studies were performed between 8:00 p.m. and 3:00 a.m. under dim scattered light since the focal animals are strictly nocturnal.

(b) Chemical transfer experiment

The chemical transfer experiment was carried out to test whether silverfish acquire CHCs from their host through physical contact. One hundred mature workers (collected from raids), 60 callows (newly hatched workers), approximately 40 larvae, 30 pupae, 21 silverfish and 10 non-myrmecophilous isopods (as a control) were kept in a nest constructed from natural materials (soil and leaf litter). The callows were treated with a stable isotope-labelled hydrocarbon (eicosane-d42, C/D/N Isotopes Inc., Canada, Pointe-Claire). Callows were selected for the labelling treatment because they are less mobile [30] and less aggressive (CvB and VW, personal observation), and silverfish were found to interact preferentially with them [26]. Eicosane-d42 was used as a label because it has properties (chain length, molecular weight) similar to the CHCs that occur naturally on the host ants [26]. 200 μl of a saturated eicosane-d42 hexane solution were evaporated in a clean 20 ml glass vial so that the hydrocarbon fully covered the bottom of the vial as a solid film. The callows were then enclosed in the vial, which was moved gently for 30 min to transfer the labelled compound. Callows did not visibly suffer from this treatment. Labelled callows, untreated workers, silverfish, and control isopods were kept three days together in the laboratory nest and subsequently they were extracted for chemical analysis (details below). The isopods, collected from the natural habitat, were added to test whether eicosane-d42 transmits to animals in the nest environment that are not specifically in close contact with the host. Preliminary studies revealed that isopods were ignored by ants, which makes them ideal control animals. Ten additional isopods were directly labelled with eicosane-d42 as described above (labelled isopods) and extracted to verify that the isopod cuticle is able to adsorb the labelled compound.

(c) Isolation treatments

To manipulate the presence of host CHC profiles on silverfish (under the assumption of acquisition through physical contact) and to test for effects on behavioural interactions with the host ants, silverfish and host ants (as experimental and control groups, respectively) were separated from their home colonies and kept isolated for six (6d) or nine days (9d) (for sample sizes refer to table 1). Isolated and non-isolated individuals as well as some resident host workers were either extracted with hexane to analyse changes in their CHCs (colonies 1-3; see section (d) of the methods) or they were tested behaviourally in their home colonies for social acceptance (colonies 4-6; see section (e) of methods), or both in combination (colony 7; see section (f) of the methods). The combined experiment (section (f) of the methods) was best suited for testing whether an individual's accuracy of chemical mimicry affects its level of social acceptance. The chemical (section (d) of the methods) and behavioural effects (section (e) of methods) were in addition studied independently, as an influence on the parasites' CHC signature through host contact during behavioural tests cannot be ruled out in the combined experiment.

Table 1 Overview of the number of silverfish individuals observed within each colony for the analysis of CHCs, the social acceptance experiment and the measurement of the silverfish' body surface area.

A control experiment was performed with silverfish to determine whether the isolation treatment itself had an effect on their social acceptance, rather than changes in their CHC signature (e.g., due to physical suffering or adoption of additional compounds that originate from the experimental setup). As before, silverfish were isolated (for six days), and then one group was directly subjected to aggression tests and subsequently to chemical analyses, while the other group was allowed an additional 24 h contact with 50 host callows, before they were tested in the same way. The latter group thereby experienced the isolation treatment but was also given the chance to re-acquire host CHCs (silverfish isolation control; colony 8). Additional isolation control experiments were performed with adult ant workers collected from raids. The ant worker controls were conducted with three different colonies (colonies 9-11). These controls intended to test whether an isolation treatment similarly affects the expression of host worker CHC profiles (given the fact that they are able to biosynthesise the CHCs).

(d) Analysis of CHCs

Specimens were transferred individually into 2 ml vials with PTFE septa and extracted for 10 min in 200 μl hexane (HPLC grade, Sigma-Aldrich). After evaporation of the solvent, the CHCs were re-dissolved in 40 μl hexane containing an internal standard (methyl stearate, FLUKA Analytics, Sigma-Aldrich), and 20 μl were transferred into a 0.3 ml vial with limited volume insert (Chromacol, 03-FISV). Using an auto sampler (Agilent technologies, 7683 Series) 1 μl of each sample was injected into a gas chromatograph (Agilent Technologies 6890N) coupled to a mass spectrometer (Agilent Technologies GC 5975 MSD). Details on the methods can be found in [28].

Chemicals were identified by mass spectra and retention indices (RI), and peak areas were extracted using the software AMDIS (version 2.68) [31]. A target library of 109 compounds was created based on the compounds found on host ants and myrmecophiles [26]. As AMDIS uses the mass spectrum as well as the retention index to identify a substance, it has the advantage of reliably detecting compounds, even in low quantities. Structural alkene isomers were distinguished although double bond positions were not determined.

The absolute quantity of each compound was calculated using the internal standard (concentration = 20 ng/μl). The resulting total quantity of a sample was divided by the animals' surface area in square millimetres in order to standardise to a presumably perceivable concentration of chemicals by an ant's antennal contact and to control for size differences between animals. To calculate surface areas, the bodies of silverfish, workers and isopods were subdivided into geometrical areas and the relevant dimensions were measured using a stereomicroscope (Zeiss Stemi 2000-C) with a measuring eyepiece (see additional file 1: Calculation of animal surface areas). The surface area of silverfish was calculated for each individual separately because they varied considerably in size, while a median surface area was used for workers as well as for isopods. Specimens were stored in pure ethanol.

(e) Social acceptance experiments

The host's aggression toward individual silverfish or individual workers was quantified through a standardised contact study in laboratory nests. The nests contained 200 ant workers, which were collected from raids, because foraging workers behave more aggressively and are thus more likely to defend the colony [32].

Furthermore, the ants were given 1 h time to settle in the laboratory nest before starting the experiments because ants tend to behave more aggressively in familiar territory than in an unfamiliar setting [33]. Fifty consecutive encounters of a silverfish individual (or worker individual) and ants were then categorized according to table 2. Each individual was tested only once. However, repeated interactions with the same ant individuals were possible. Nevertheless, since we focused on a colony-level defence, which naturally includes task allocation, repeated interactions of the same workers do not affect our interpretations. An aggression index (AI) was calculated for each silverfish from the observed interactions as follows: AI = NA/NT with NA = number of aggressive interactions and NT = total number of interactions.

Table 2 Behavioural interactions between silverfish and ants and behavioural categories used for calculating the aggression index.

Some silverfish (N = 14) were seized by the ants before 50 encounters were completed. These individuals were removed to prevent their destruction so that they could be used for chemical analysis and body measurements. Although these individuals did not reach 50 host encounters, their AI was calculated as described above.

(f) Accuracy of chemical mimicry

To directly test the relation of chemical host resemblance to social acceptance for the same individuals, we combined the social acceptance study and the analysis of CHCs in one L. distinguenda colony (colony 7; table 1). For each silverfish individual host aggression was quantified first via social acceptance experiments (standardised contact study), and then its CHCs were extracted and subjected to chemical analysis.

(g) Data analysis

Chemical and behavioural data were evaluated with the software PRIMER 6 (version 6.1.12, Primer-E Ltd., Ivybridge, U.K.) with the PERMANOVA+ add-in (version 1.0.2) using a non-parametric permutational analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) with 9,999 permutations [34]. PERMANOVA models were based on Bray-Curtis similarities (as a semi-metric measure), either calculated from a single response variable (chemical similarity, CHC concentration, aggression index), or from numerous response variables (CHC profiles, behavioural interactions). Nonmetric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) was used to visualise multivariate data (PRIMER 6). Box plots were created from univariate data with the Microsoft Excel add-in SSC-Stat (version 2.18, Statistical service centre of the University of Reading, Reading, U.K.). Chi-square tests were accomplished using XLSTAT (Version 2010.3.06, Addinsoft, U.S.A.).

Chemical analysis

Since no silverfish-specific compounds were detected, the principle compounds that together constituted 99% to the chemical profiles of workers (N = 44) according to a similarity percentage analysis (SIMPER) on Bray-Curtis similarities were included in the statistical analysis of the CHC composition, the presence or absence of CHCs, the total CHC concentration and the chemical similarity (N = 32 compounds; see additional file 2: Table of compounds). To test whether the chemical similarity of silverfish to their host colony was influenced by isolation treatments, Bray-Curtis similarities to the average worker CHC profile of the respective host colony were used as a univariate response variable, and a PERMANOVA with a 2-factor nested design (colonies (random), days of isolation (fixed), nested in colony) was applied. No chemical worker profiles were available for colony 3. To test for additional differences in the quantity of CHCs, absolute concentrations (per surface area) were analysed in the same way.

Furthermore, multivariate approaches were used to analyse relative changes in CHC composition (Bray-Curtis similarities), and the presence or absence of compounds (simple matching). A PERMANOVA with a 2-factor nested design as described above was applied for both analyses. Chromatograms of chemical profiles of host ants and silverfish can be found in an earlier article of one of the authors [26].

Behavioural analysis

Aggression indices of isolated vs. non-isolated individuals were compared using a PERMANOVA with a two-factor nested design as described above. The interactions of silverfish with their host ants were evaluated in a multivariate approach including all observed behaviours. These were standardised by the total number of interactions and a 2-factor nested design as described above was applied.

Results

(a) Chemical transfer experiment

Previously labelled callows still carried high concentrations of eicosane-d42 after the three-day experimental phase (median = 46.18 ng/mm2; Figure 2). Interestingly, the concentration of eicosane-d42 did not differ between silverfish (median = 44.57 ng/mm2) and callows (median = 46.18 ng/mm2; PERMANOVA, P = 0.986), while lower concentrations were found on adult workers (median = 10.60 ng/mm2; PERMANOVA, for both comparisons P < 0.001). Almost no eicosane-d42 was found on control isopods (median = 0 ng/mm2), which consequently differed from labelled callows, workers and silverfish (PERMANOVA, for all comparisons P < 0.001). High quantities of eicosane-d42 on the labelled isopods (median = 100.13 ng/mm2) demonstrated that their cuticle has the potential to adsorb the labelled CHC.

Figure 2
figure 2

Concentrations of eicosane-d 42 in the CHC transfer experiment. Different capital letters show significant differences (P < 0.05) between groups evaluated by PERMANOVA. Median (red cross = mean), quartiles (boxes), 10th and 90th percentiles (whiskers), and outliers (black square = outlier, asterisk = extreme point) are shown.

(b) Analysis of CHCs

Seventy compounds were detected on workers (N = 44). No silverfish-specific CHC was found (N = 133). The number of detected host compounds on silverfish decreased after isolation treatments (no isolation: Ncompounds = 28, Nsilverfish = 63; 6 days isolation: Ncompounds = 22, Nsilverfish = 12; 9 days isolation: Ncompounds = 23, Nsilverfish = 58). No compounds were detected on some of the specimens that had been isolated for 9 days (5 out of 58).

Non-isolated silverfish were chemically closer to their host workers than isolated individuals (PERMANOVA, for all colonies P ≤ 0.025; Figure 3). Significant differences between isolated and non-isolated silverfish were detected in the relative composition, the presence or absence, and in the total concentration of CHCs in three out of four different colonies (table 3). In colony 3 there was a trend that the compositions of CHCs differed between non-isolated and isolated silverfish (PERMANOVA, P = 0.064), whereas the presence or absence of CHCs and the CHC concentrations did not differ (PERMANOVA, P ≥ 0.134). For this colony we had the smallest sample size (see table 1).

Figure 3
figure 3

Chemical similarities of individual silverfish to the average chemical worker profile of their host colony ( N workers ≥ 10). No chemical worker profiles were available for colony 3. Differences between isolated vs. non-isolated silverfish were evaluated by PERMANOVA (*P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001). Median (red cross = mean), quartiles (boxes) 10th and 90th percentiles (whiskers), and outliers (black square = outlier; asterisk = extreme point) are shown. Abbreviations: No iso = no isolation, d iso = days isolated.

Table 3 Comparison of non-isolated silverfish (0 d) and isolated silverfish (6 d or 9 d) regarding their CHC composition, presence or absence of CHCs and their total CHC concentration.

Non-isolated silverfish carried higher total concentrations of host CHCs on their body (median = 55.23 ng/mm2, N = 40) than isolated silverfish (median 6 days = 10.95 ng/mm2, N = 12; median 9 days = 13.98 ng/mm2, N = 42; PERMANOVA, P < 0.001; for within colony comparisons see additional file 3: Concentrations of CHCs). Workers carried significantly higher concentrations than both silverfish groups (median = 106.23 ng/mm2, N = 44; PERMANOVA, P < 0.001). Across all colonies the median concentration of every compound was lower after isolation.

(c) Social acceptance experiment

In all colonies, isolated silverfish were treated with higher aggression by host workers than non-isolated silverfish (PERMANOVA, for all comparisons P ≤ 0.004; Figure 4). The higher aggression toward isolated silverfish was also reflected in the frequency with which they were seized by workers. Only 4% of non-isolated silverfish were seized, while 26% of the six day isolated and 20% of nine-day isolated individuals were seized. All isolated silverfish were seized by workers in colony 6. The frequencies of seized and non-seized silverfish did not differ between six and nine days isolated silverfish (Chi square test: χ2 = 0.232, df = 1, P = 0.630, N 1 = 38, N 2 = 15), but they differed significantly between non-isolated and isolated individuals (Chi square test: χ2 = 11.851, df = 1, P = 0.001, N 1 = 53, N 2 = 53).

Figure 4
figure 4

Host aggression in three different colonies toward non-isolated silverfish and silverfish that were isolated for six days. Differences between groups were evaluated by PERMANOVA (**P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001). Median (red cross = mean), quartiles (boxes), 10th and 90th percentiles (whiskers), and outliers (black square = outlier) are shown. Abbreviations: No iso = no isolation, d iso = days isolated.

Considering the multivariate analysis of all behavioural interactions, we found significant differences in three of four colonies (colony 4, 5 and 7) between non-isolated and isolated silverfish (PERMANOVA, for all pair-wise comparisons P ≤ 0.012). Colony 6 was not evaluated because all of the isolated individuals were seized by worker ants and could not complete the standardised number of 50 ant contacts. For detailed information on behavioural interactions across all colonies see additional file 4: Behavioral interactions.

(d) Accuracy of chemical mimicry

In the experiment on the accuracy of chemical mimicry, the cuticular profile of isolated silverfish was also less similar to host workers (PERMANOVA, P < 0.001) and the same silverfish individuals received more aggression in contact studies than non-isolated individuals did (PERMANOVA, P ≤ 0.004; Figure 5). As in the experiments described above the total concentration of CHCs was lower in isolated silverfish (median = 4.51 ng/mm2, N = 21; PERMANOVA, P < 0.001) than in non-isolated silverfish (median = 27.66 ng/mm2, N = 21). Furthermore, non-isolated silverfish remained unnoticed more often and were ignored more frequently compared to isolated individuals (see additional files 4 and 5). Isolated silverfish were more frequently antennated by host workers, and they avoided host contact more often than non-isolated silverfish. Most importantly, ant workers chased and snapped at isolated silverfish more frequently than at non-isolated silverfish. There were no significant differences in the interactions "groomed" (PERMANOVA, P = 0.364) and "stung" (PERMANOVA, P = 0.365) between isolated and non-isolated silverfish (see additional file 4).

Figure 5
figure 5

Chemical similarities of silverfish to the average chemical worker profile (N workers = 19; left) and host aggression toward the same individuals (right) in colony 7. Differences between isolated and non-isolated silverfish were evaluated by PERMANOVA (**P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001). Median (red cross = mean), quartiles (boxes), 10th and 90th percentiles (whiskers), and outliers (black square = outlier; asterisk = extreme point) are shown. Abbreviations: No iso = no isolation, d iso = days isolated.

In the isolation control experiment silverfish that were first isolated for six days and then kept together with callows for 24 h showed greater chemical similarity (PERMANOVA, P < 0.001) and were treated less aggressively (PERMANOVA, P < 0.001) than individuals that were only isolated but had no secondary contact to the host (see additional file 6: Silverfish isolation control experiment). In the host worker isolation control experiment, CHC concentration did not decrease after isolation, instead it increased in two colonies (additional file 7: Worker control experiment). Furthermore, aggressive behaviour did not increase (no isolation: three aggressive interactions from a total of 2282 recorded interactions; nine days isolation: five aggressive interactions from a total of 2777 recorded interactions).

Discussion

The present study sheds light on two important aspects of social insect parasitism. Our results strongly indicate that mimetic CHCs are acquired by a parasite from the cuticle of its host and that higher accuracy in mimicking host CHCs can be crucial for social exploitation due to the avoidance of aggressive rejection. In the following paragraphs we discuss in detail the integration mechanism of the parasitic silverfish M. ponerophila.

Origin of mimetic compounds (acquisition vs. biosynthesis)

The adoption of a stable isotope-labelled hydrocarbon from the cuticle of the host by the silverfish but not by control animals indicates that silverfish use a behavioural mechanism for acquiring mimetic CHCs, rather than innate biosynthesis. Eicosane-d42 has properties (chain length, molecular weight) similar to CHCs that occur naturally on the host ants, thus we conclude that the hosts' natural surface compounds are acquired by the same mechanism. Although we did not directly tested natural host CHCs, these compounds are most likely transferred in the same way. We cannot imagine a mechanism by which silverfish acquire selectively only particular compounds from the host cuticle. Furthermore, the mechanism of pilfering host CHCs (e.g., rubbing behaviour [26]; see also additional file 8: Video of M. ponerophila) appears to be very effective, as silverfish accumulated even higher concentrations of eicosane-d42 from the labelled callows than host workers did. In agreement with a behavioural adoption, the mimetic CHCs on silverfish decreased in isolation treatments quantitatively (total concentration) and qualitatively (relative abundance and presence or absence). Importantly, mimetic CHCs increased again after secondary contact of previously isolated silverfish with host ants. Taken together, the loss of mimetic cues after isolation and their re-occurrence after secondary host contact point strongly towards an effective behavioural acquisition of host CHCs. Alternatively, these findings could be explained by a context-specific up- and down-regulation of CHC biosynthesis in the silverfish. However, due to the direct transfer of the labelled compound from host to silverfish, and due to evolutionary considerations that we explain below, this seems highly unlikely.

The exchange of surface compounds through physical contact (trophallaxis, allogrooming and/or other contact) has previously been demonstrated to occur among ant nestmates [35] but not between host ants and their myrmecophiles. Even though previous studies have been founded on the assumption that myrmecophiles acquire rather than synthesise mimetic compounds to achieve chemical resemblance [36–38], acquisition has never been clearly demonstrated. A loss of host-specific surface compounds after isolation has already been demonstrated in the beetle Myrmecaphodius excavaticollis [36] as well as in the cricket Myrmecophilus sp. [39]. These results render the biosynthesis of host CHCs in these myrmecophiles unlikely, but a potential ability to down-regulate the biosynthesis of host-specific CHCs in the absence of a host cannot be ruled out. Such ability was found in the myrmecophilous butterfly Phengaris (Maculinea) rebeli [40].

Phengaris (Maculinea) rebeli caterpillars biosynthesise a subset of their host's hydrocarbons to become attractive, resulting in the transport into the nests [41]. Importantly, for this mechanism to work, the allomones produced by innate biosynthesis must be colony-unspecific. Indeed, Phengaris (Maculinea) caterpillars seem to mimic the surface chemistry of ant brood [41, 42], which is generally less complex compared to that of workers and is assumed to be colony-unspecific. Hence, appropriate cues may mimic, for example, certain key stimuli of brood or males [38, 40]. We presume that the more complex a host's recognition signature is, the more difficult it becomes for distantly related organisms to evolve the appropriate biosynthetic pathways for the production of the essential recognition cues and to express the compounds in the correct relative proportions (even if key regulatory enzymes are involved). In such cases, mixed strategies or the adoption of recognition cues may be evolutionarily more parsimonious. Another problem associated with the biosynthesis of mimetic cues is the dynamic nature of colony specific CHC profiles. An ant species is typically characterized by a set of CHCs, which differ among colonies in relative proportions [9]. Hydrocarbons are exchanged between nestmates by means of trophallaxis (exchange of nutritional liquids between nestmates) and allogrooming (grooming directed towards a nestmate), which establishes a uniform colony odour-the "gestalt odour" [9, 43]. Despite its uniformity, the "gestalt odour" changes over time due to factors such as shifts in diet [44, 45], different nesting materials [46] or seasonal differences [47]. Biosynthesis of worker CHCs is unlikely to be able to adjust to such flexible but specific "gestalt odours". These considerations may explain why an acquisition of mimetic CHCs is found more frequently than biosynthesis among distantly related parasites of social insects.

The role of accuracy in chemical mimicry

In addition to the mechanism of acquired chemical mimicry, our results highlight the importance of accuracy in chemical host resemblance by demonstrating that aggressive rejection can be avoided through closer chemical resemblance to the host. Notably, a parasites' successful social integration by chemical mimicry needs to include in principle only the cues that are necessary for nestmate recognition and not all the host CHCs. Nestmate recognition in the ant species Formica exsecta, for example, seems to be based only on selected compounds [48]. All types of compounds present on the cuticles of the host ant L. distinguenda and its parasitic silverfish could potentially be involved in recognition. Due to the generally accepted role of CHCs in ant nestmate recognition, we focused on non-polar compounds by using an appropriate solvent. There were several host CHCs on the silverfish, but only traces of other compounds (see additional file 2). Since host aggressiveness apparently depended on the chemical similarity of silverfish to their host, we conclude that the chemical recognition of silverfish by the host is predominantly based on CHCs. However, we were not able to differentiate which characteristics of CHC profiles, i.e. the composition (relative proportions), the presence or absence or the concentrations play the major role in the recognition of the silverfish.

A relationship between chemical resemblance and aggression is well known in the nestmate recognition of ants. Workers of the Argentine ant Linepithema humile showed elevated aggression against conspecific workers that were chemically more distant, while conspecific workers with similar profiles were treated amicably [49]. Among myrmecophiles, the Phengaris (Maculinea) alcon caterpillar biosynthesises a "pre-adoption" profile and adoption of caterpillars happened faster with higher accuracy of chemical mimicry of the host [42]. The innate biosynthesis of CHCs by myrmecophiles means that the origins of mimetic CHCs and model CHCs are different, which allows coevolutionary arms races to shape the degree of mimicry as well as the discrimination abilities by ants [50]. As described above, the synthesis of particular key stimuli used to deceive the host may be selected for in these scenarios [50]. These colony-unspecific stimuli allow the caterpillars to be adopted by any colony of their respective hosts, accompanied by local adaptation on a population level. In contrast, acquisition through physical contact to the host, as demonstrated here, means that the mimetic compounds of the model and the mimic are of identical origin. Coevolutionary arms races operate differently in this case, selecting for mimics with effective ways of acquiring host CHCs (e.g., through rubbing behaviour in M. ponerophila or the consumption of host larvae in Cosmophasis bitaeniata [51]). In the host, selection favours defence mechanisms to prevent such "CHC pilfering" by parasitic myrmecophiles [52]. The present study indicates that such a coevolutionary arms race takes place between the host L. distinguenda and the myrmecophile M. ponerophila. Sufficient contacts between the silverfish and the host ants are required to refresh the mimetic compounds and to gain increased chemical resemblance to the host, in order to acquire the colony's current "gestalt odour", resulting in social acceptance.

Besides adaptive adjustments in mimetic CHCs, the presence of additional cuticular compounds that do not match the ants' current template could potentially be responsible for recognition of aliens in social insect societies, and such cues could also explain the observed attacks against isolated silverfish. Workers of the carpenter ant Camponotus herculeanus, for example, attacked nestmates if they possessed one additional, foreign compound on their cuticle [45]. However, in our experiments an acquisition of additional compounds during isolation treatments that could have been responsible for the observed aggression seems unlikely for several reasons. First, aggression towards isolated and non-isolated host workers was not different, indicating that their chemical profiles were not influenced by the treatment. Second, we did not detect any specific compounds in silverfish (neither among isolated nor among non-isolated individuals) that could be responsible for the aggression, but we cannot exclude effects of compounds that were undetectable by the GC-MS analysis that was used. However, the silverfish isolation control experiment (additional file 6) finally shows that isolated silverfish did not acquire additional compounds during isolation that elicit aggression. Individuals that were first isolated and then were given the chance to re-acquire host CHCs were attacked significantly less than silverfish that were only isolated, indicating that only mimetic host compounds were behaviourally active.

Behavioural and morphological adaptations

Considering the level of integration a myrmecophile can achieve, we want to emphasise that mechanisms other than chemical integration may also play important roles, such as acoustic mimicry or behavioural and morphological adaptations [20, 21, 53]. The myrmecophilous cricket Myrmecophilous formosanus, for example, avoids ant attacks by swift movements [53]. Malayatelura ponerophila was also regularly observed escaping by quick movements (behavioural category "avoid" in additional file 4). About 75% of the isolated silverfish survived the observation period despite frequent ant attacks during escape. The limuloid (drop-shaped), scaled body of silverfish, with short appendages (antennae, cerci and praecerci) and retractable head, may also facilitate escaping ant attacks. The convergent evolution of the limuloid body form in unrelated myrmecophilous taxa provides strong evidence of its adaptive value [54]. These traits may also help M. ponerophila to survive ant attacks in natural nests and perhaps offer the possibility of invading new host colonies, albeit this is presumably a risky manoeuvre. Malayatelura ponerophila usually prefers central regions within natural nests where callows, pupae and larvae are located [26]. When individuals are able to reach this inner part of the nest, they are in a fairly safe place, which not only offers shelter and food, but also offers the possibility to steal the host's chemical profile by rubbing their surface on defenceless callows (see additional file 8: Video of M. ponerophila).

Conclusion

In summary, we show that ant parasites can acquire CHCs directly from their host. Although elaborate behavioural adaptations may be required, the direct acquisition of host CHCs appears to be an evolutionarily parsimonious mechanism for taxonomically distant parasites such as M. ponerophila. Furthermore, our study reveals that the accuracy of chemical mimicry can be crucial for parasites of social insects to gain social acceptance. For M. ponerophila, regular replenishment of mimetic compounds increases survival because individuals with low chemical host resemblance are recognised and attacked frequently, sometimes captured and killed. Notably, the less frequently a silverfish replenishes its chemical profile (e.g., by failure to locate defenceless callows), the more difficult it becomes to remain unrecognised and to seek contact with the host ants.

References

  1. Dawkins R, Krebs JR: Arms races between and within species. Proc R Soc B. 1979, 205: 489-511. 10.1098/rspb.1979.0081.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Combes C: Arms races. The art of being a parasite. Edited by: Combes C. 2005, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 8-20.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Combes C: The art of being a parasite. 2005, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  4. Moore J: Parasites and the behavior of animals. 2002, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  5. Wilson EO: Success and dominance in ecosystems: the case of the social insects. 1990, Excellence in ecology, Oldendorf/Luhe: Ecology Institute

    Google Scholar 

  6. Hölldobler B, Wilson EO: The ants. 1990, Cambridge: Harvard University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  7. Vander Meer RK, Morel L: Nestmate recognition in ants. Pheromone communication in social insects: Ants, Wasps, Bees and Termites. Edited by: Vander Meer RK, Breed M, Winston M, Espelie KE. 1998, Boulder:Westview Press, 79-103.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Hefetz A: The evolution of hydrocarbon pheromone parsiomony in ants (Hymenoptera:Formicidae)-interplay of colony odor uniformity and odor idiosyncrasy. A review. Myrmecol News. 2007, 10: 59-68.

    Google Scholar 

  9. van Zweden JS, d'Ettorre P: Nestmate recognition in social insects and the role of hydrocarbons. Insect hydrocarbons: Biology, Biochemistry and Chemical Ecology. Edited by: Blomquist GJ, Bagnères A.-G. 2010, New York: Cambridge University Press, 222-243.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  10. Lenoir A, D'Ettorre P, Errard C, Hefetz A: Chemical ecology and social parasitism in ants. Annu Rev Entomol. 2001, 46: 573-99. 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.573.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Blomquist GJ, Bagnères A.-G: Insect Hydrocarbons: Biology, Biochemistry and Chemical Ecology. 2010, New York: Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Thomas JA, Schönrogge K, Elmes GW: Specialisation and host association of social parasites of ants. Insect Evolutionary Ecology. Edited by: Fellowes M, Hollowat G, Rolff J. 2005, London: CABI Publishing, 475-514.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Schmid-Hempel P: Parasites in social insects. 1998, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  14. Boomsma JJ, Schmid-Hempel P, Hughes WOH: Life histories and parasite pressure across the major groups of social insects. Insect Evolutionary Ecology. Edited by: Fellowes MDE, Holloway GJ, Rolff J. 2005, Wellingford: CABI Publishing, 139-175.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Wilson EO: The insect societies. 1971, Cambridge: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gösswald K: Gäste der Ameisen. Unsere Ameisen II. Edited by: Gösswald K. 1955, Stuttgart: KOSMOS

    Google Scholar 

  17. Akre RD, Rettenmeyer CW: Behavior of Staphylinidae associated with army ants (Formicidae:Ecitoninae). J Kans Entomol Soc. 1966, 39 (4): 745-782.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Kistner DH: Social and evolutionary significance of social insect symbionts. Social insects. Edited by: Hermann HR. 1979, New York: Academic press, 339-413.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Dettner K, Liepert C: Chemical mimicry and camouflage. Annu Rev Entomol. 1994, 39: 129-54. 10.1146/annurev.en.39.010194.001021.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Barbero F, Thomas JA, Bonelli S, Balletto E, Schönrogge K: Queen ants make distinctive sounds that are mimicked by a butterfly social parasite. Science. 2009, 323 (5915): 782-785. 10.1126/science.1163583.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Dinter K, Paarmann W, Peschke K, Arndt E: Ecological, behavioural and chemical adaptations to ant predation in species of Thermophilum and Graphipterus (Coleoptera: Carabidae) in the Sahara desert. J Arid Environ. 2002, 50 (2): 267-286. 10.1006/jare.2001.0850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bagnères A-G, Lorenzi M: Chemical deception/mimicry using cuticular hydrocarbons. Insect hydrocarbons: Biology, Biochemistry and Chemical Ecology. Edited by: Blomquist GJ, Bagnères A-G. 2010, New York: Cambridge University Press, 282-324.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  23. Akino T: Chemical strategies to deal with ants: a review of mimicry, camouflage, propaganda, and phytomimesis by ants (Hymenoptera:Formicidae) and other arthropods. Myrmecol News. 2008, 11: 173-181.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Howard RW, Akre RD, Garnett WB: Chemical mimicry in an obligate predator of carpenter ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1990, 83: 607-616.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Vane-Wright RI: On the definition of mimicry. Biol J Linn Soc. 1980, 13: 1-6. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1980.tb00066.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Witte V, Foitzik S, Hashim R, Maschwitz U, Schulz S: Fine tuning of social integration by two myrmecophiles of the ponerine army ant Leptogenys distinguenda. J Chem Ecol. 2009, 35 (3): 355-367. 10.1007/s10886-009-9606-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Mendes L, von Beeren C, Witte V: Malayatelura ponerophila - a new genus and species of silverfish (Zygentoma, Insecta) from Malaysia, living in Leptogenys army-ant colonies (Formicidae). Deut Entomol Z.

  28. Witte V, Leingärtner A, Sabaß L, Hashim R, Foitzik S: Symbiont microcosm in an ant society and the diversity of interspecific interactions. Anim Behav. 2008, 76: 1477-1486. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Steghaus-Kovac S: Wanderjäger im Regenwald-Lebensstrategien im Vergleich: Ökologie und Verhalten südostasiatischer Ameisenarten der Gattung Leptogenys (Hymenoptera: Formicidae: Ponerinae). PhD thesis. 1994, Frankfurt am Main: Johann Wolfgang Goethe Universität

    Google Scholar 

  30. Jaisson P: Social behavior. Comprehensive insect physiology, biochemistry and pharmacology. Edited by: Kerkut GA, Gilbert LI. 1985, Oxford: Pergamon Press, 673-694.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Stein SE: An integrated method for spectrum extraction and compound identification from GC/MS data. J Am Soc Mass Spectrom. 1999, 10: 770-781. 10.1016/S1044-0305(99)00047-1.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Witte V: Organisation und Steuerung des Treiberameisenverhaltens bei südostasiatischen Ponerinen der Gattung Leptogenys. 2001, Dissertation, Johann Wolfgang Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, [http://publikationen.ub.uni-frankfurt.de/files/5541/00000172.pdf]

    Google Scholar 

  33. Tanner CJ, Adler FR: To fight or not to fight: context-dependent interspecific aggression in competing ants. Anim Behav. 2009, 77: 297-305. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Anderson MJ, Gorley RN, Clarke KR: PERMANOVA+ for PRIMER: Guide to software and statistical methods. 2008, Plymouth: PRIMER-E

    Google Scholar 

  35. Lenoir A, Hefetz A, Simon T, Soroker V: Comparative dynamics of gestalt odour formation in two ant species Camponotus fellah and Aphaenogaster senilis (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Physiol Entomol. 2001, 26: 275-83. 10.1046/j.0307-6962.2001.00244.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Vander Meer RK, Wojcik DP: Chemical mimicry in the myrmecophilous beetle Myrmecaphodius excavaticollis. Science. 1982, 218: 806-808. 10.1126/science.218.4574.806.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Vander Meer RK, Jouvenaz DP, Wojcik DP: Chemical mimicry in a parasitoid (Hymenoptera: Eucharitidae) of fire ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). J Chem Ecol. 1989, 15: 2247-61. 10.1007/BF01014113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hojo MK, Wada-Katsumata A, Akino T, Yamaguchi S, Ozaki M, Yamaoka R: Chemical disguise as particular caste of host ants in the ant inquiline parasite Niphanda fusca (Lepidoptera: Lycaenidae). Proc R Soc B. 2009, 276 (1656): 551-558. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1064.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Akino T, Mochizuki R, Morimoto M, Yamaoka R: Chemical camouflage of myrmecophilous cricket Myrmecophilus sp. to be integrated with several ant species. Jpn J Appl Entomol Zool. 1996, 40: 39-46. 10.1303/jjaez.40.39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Schönrogge K, Wardlaw JC, Peters AJ, Everett S, Thomas JA, Elmes GW: Changes in chemical signature and host specificity from larval retrieval to full social integration in the myrmecophilous butterfly Maculinea rebeli. J Chem Ecol. 2004, 30: 91-107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Akino T, Knapp JJ, Thomas JA, Elmes GW: Chemical mimicry and host specificity in the butterfly Maculinea rebeli, a social parasite of Myrmica ant colonies. Proc R Soc B. 1999, 266: 1419-1426. 10.1098/rspb.1999.0796.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Nash DR, Als TD, Maile R, Jones GR, Boomsma JJ: A mosaic of chemical coevolution in a large blue butterfly. Science. 2008, 319 (5859): 88-90. 10.1126/science.1149180.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Crozier RH, Dix MW: Analysis of two genetic models for the innate components of colony odor in social Hymenoptera. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1979, 4: 217-224. 10.1007/BF00297645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Liang D, Silverman J: "You are what you eat": Diet modifies cuticular hydrocarbons and nestmate recognition in the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile. Naturwissenschaften. 2000, 87: 412-416. 10.1007/s001140050752.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Guerrieri FJ, Nehring V, Jørgensen CG, Nielsen J, Galizia CG, d'Ettorre P: Ants recognize foes and not friends. Proc R Soc B. 2009, 276: 2461-2468. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1860.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Heinze J, Foitzik S, Hippert A, Hölldobler B: Apparent dear-enemy phenomenon and environment-based recognition cues in the ant Leptothorax nylanderi. Ethology. 1996, 102: 510-22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Vander Meer RK, Saliwanchik D, Lavine B: Temporal changes in colony cuticular hydrocarbon patterns of Solenopsis invicta: Implications for nestmate recognition. J Chem Ecol. 1989, 15 (7): 2115-2126. 10.1007/BF01207442.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Martin SJ, Vitikainen E, Helanterä Drijfhout F: Chemical basis of nest-mate discrimination in the ant Formica exsecta. Proc R Soc B. 2008, 275: 1271-1278. 10.1098/rspb.2007.1708.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. van Wilgenburg E, Torres CW, Tsutsui ND: The global expansion of a single ant supercolony. Evol Appl. 2010, 3 (2): 136-143. 10.1111/j.1752-4571.2009.00114.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Nash A, Boomsma J: Communiction between hosts and social parasites. Sociobiology of communication an interdisciplinary approach. Edited by: d'Ettorre P, Hughes DP. 2008, New York: Oxford University Press, 55-79.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  51. Elgar MA, Allan RA: Predatory spider mimics acquire colony-specific cuticular hydrocarbons from their ant model prey. Naturwissenschaften. 2004, 91: 143-147. 10.1007/s00114-004-0507-y.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. von Beeren C, Maruyama M, Hashim R, Witte V: Differential host defense against multiple parasites in ants. Evol Ecol. 2011, 25: 259-276. 10.1007/s10682-010-9420-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Komatsu T, Maruyama M, Itino T: Behavioral differences between two ant cricket species in Nansei Islands: host-specialist versus host-generalist. Insect Soc. 2009, 56 (4): 389-396. 10.1007/s00040-009-0036-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gotwald WH: Army ants: the biology of social predation. 1995, Ithaca: Cornell University Press

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the behavioural ecology group at the LMU Munich for helpful comments on the manuscript, with special thanks to Sebastian Pohl. Many thanks go to Sofia Lizon à l'Allemand, Max Kölbl, Magdalena Mair, Hannah Kriesell and Deborah Schweinfest for their valuable assistance in the field. We are grateful for financial support from the DFG (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, project WI 2646/3) and for helpful comments of three anonymous referees.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Volker Witte.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

CvB and VW designed the study, acquired the data, analysed and interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. SS identified the chemical compounds and revised the manuscript. R H revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12898_2011_189_MOESM1_ESM.PDF

Additional file 1: Calculation of animal surface areas. Calculation of surface areas of the bodies of silverfish, workers and isopods. (PDF 303 KB)

12898_2011_189_MOESM2_ESM.PDF

Additional file 2: Table of compounds. Concentrations of 32 compounds that constituted 99.06% of the chemical profiles of workers (N = 44) across colonies evaluated by a similarity percentage analysis (SIMPER) on Bray-Curtis distances. In addition, concentrations of non-isolated (Sf 0 d; N = 63), six days isolated (Sf 6 d; N = 12) and nine day isolated silverfish (Sf 9 d; N = 56) across colonies are shown. (PDF 88 KB)

12898_2011_189_MOESM3_ESM.PDF

Additional file 3: Concentration of CHCs. Shown is the total quantity of surface chemicals per area of non-isolated and isolated silverfish. (PDF 189 KB)

12898_2011_189_MOESM4_ESM.PDF

Additional file 4: Behavioural interactions in the social acceptance experiment. Detailed information on behavioural interactions between silverfish and host ants across all colonies. (PDF 76 KB)

12898_2011_189_MOESM5_ESM.PDF

Additional file 5: NMDS plot of behavioural interactions between isolated and non-isolated silverfish and their host ants for colony 7. (PDF 194 KB)

12898_2011_189_MOESM6_ESM.PDF

Additional file 6: Silverfish isolation control experiment. Control for isolation treatment. Chemical similarities of silverfish to host workers and aggression toward the same individuals in colony 8. (PDF 183 KB)

Additional file 7: Worker control experiment. Concentration of CHCs on non-isolated and isolated workers. (PDF 194 KB)

12898_2011_189_MOESM8_ESM.MPEG

Additional file 8: Video of M. ponerophila. The video shows M. ponerophila together with host workers and brood in an artificial nest site. One silverfish individual (in the foreground) rubs its own body intensely on that of an ant worker, presumably to acquire host CHCs. (MPEG 3 MB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

von Beeren, C., Schulz, S., Hashim, R. et al. Acquisition of chemical recognition cues facilitates integration into ant societies. BMC Ecol 11, 30 (2011). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1472-6785-11-30

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1472-6785-11-30

Keywords